Prefrontal Cortex Response to Threat: Race by Age Variation in 9-10 Year Old Children

Shervin Assari1,2*, Golnoush Akhlaghipour3, Mohammed Saqib4, Shanika Boyce5, Mohsen Bazargan1

1Department of Family Medicine, Charles R. Drew University of Medicine and Science, Los Angeles, CA, USA

2Department of Urban Public Health, Charles R. Drew University of Medicine and Science, Los Angeles, CA, USA

3Department of Neurology, UCLA, Los Angeles, CA, USA

4Department of Health Behavior and Health Education, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA

5Department of Pediatrics, Charles R. Drew University of Medicine and Science, Los Angeles, CA, USA


Background: Considerable research has suggested that race and age are two major determinants of brain development, including but not limited to development of the prefrontal cortex (PFC). Minorities’ Diminished Returns (MDRs), however, suggests that race (as a proxy of racism) may interact with various determinants of human and brain development. Minimal knowledge, however, exists on whether age and race also interact on shaping PFC response to threat among American children.

Purpose: Using data from a task-based functional brain imaging study and considering race as a sociological rather than a biological construct, we investigated combined effects of race and age on prefrontal cortical (PFC) response to threat. We explored racial heterogeneities in the association between age and PFC response to threat by comparing Black and White children.

Methods: This study used the task-based functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) data from the Adolescents Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study, a national, landmark, multi-center brain imaging investigation of 9-10 years old children in the US. The primary outcomes were mean beta weights of n-back runs measuring PFC response to threating versus neutral face contrast in the following regions of interest (ROIs): left hemisphere-lateral orbito-frontal, left hemisphere -superior-frontal, right hemisphere -caudal middle frontal, and right hemisphere -superior frontal cortex. The independent variable was age. Covariates were sex, ethnicity, family socioeconomic status, and neighborhood socioeconomic status. Race was the focal moderator. To analyze the data, we used linear regression models without and with interactions and SES as covariates.

Results: We included 5,066 9-10 years old children. Age and race did not show direct effects on PFC response to threatening relative to neutral faces. While ethnicity, sex, and socioeconomic status were controlled, age and race showed a systematic interaction on PFC response to threatening relative to neutral faces.

Conclusions: For American children, race and age do not have direct effects but multiplicative effects on PFC response to threat. The results may be reflective of social inequalities in how Black and White children are socialized and developed. The results are important given the role of the PFC in regulating the limbic system response to threat. Coordinated work of the limbic system and PFC is a core element of children’s behavioral and emotional development. Future research is needed on how social stratification and racism shape emotion processing and regulation of American children in response to threat.


Age is one of the most salient determinants of child brain development1-4. While early childhood5,6 and infancy1,7,8 are also important, adolescence is a unique phase determined by a rapid change of brain maturation9. The National Institute of Health (NIH) notes about child brain development that: 1) the brain reaches its biggest size in early adolescence (11 and 14 years old for girls and boys respectively), 2) the prefrontal cortex, undergoes maturation later than other parts of the brain, 3) a teenager's brain has considerable plasticity and resilience, meaning that it can successfully adapt and respond to its environment, 4) undergoes ongoing and rapid changes with, 5) co-occurring physical, emotional, and social changes, and 6) exhibits the highest vulnerability to stress9.

Response to threat10-12, one of the major functions of the brain, has major implications for survival as well as social relations. Research has well shown that a large array of cortical and subcortical brain structures are involved in processing threat- and fear-related information13-16, all being essential for maintaining social behavior, group affiliation, and attachment17. To increase survival, the brain needs to strongly respond to threat, as it encounters and processes negative stimuli (e.g. threat or fear related), as well as responding to stimuli that are unpredictable, ambiguous, and potentially dangerous18. An increased response to threat is also a part of psychopathologies such as anxiety19, depression20-23, poor decision making24-26, aggression27,28, and risk behaviors29. Research has shown that age30-32, race33-38, and socioeconomic status (SES)39-41 are among the main predictors of the brain response to threat.

While age may alter how the brain evaluates and responds to threat, it is not the only factor. Race, family socioeconomic status, are all proxies of exposure to stress, adversities, trauma, and all other salient determinants of children brain response to threat39-43. As social adversities, stress, and trauma impact brain response to threat43, there is a need to control if race and age still impact brain response to threat after socioeconomic status (as a proxy of stress and trauma) is controlled for.

Most of the existing research on the effect of age on brain response to threat is performed in White middle-class individuals. Research has, however, shown that what applied to the White middle-class may not necessarily apply to Blacks and other racial and ethnic groups. Any enhancement of these studies, however, require a large sample size. Such sample size requires large scale national studies that enhance statistical power for racial comparisons and also generate results that are generalizable to the US population. The Adolescents Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD), a national children’s brain development study data with 10,000+ sample size44-48 has generated an unprecedented opportunity to investigate how racial groups differ in the effects of risk and protective factors on brain development. As such, the ABCD study has also provided us a unique opportunity for comparison of Black and White children for age-related changes in those who live in low socioeconomic status families and neighborhoods. ABCD has a large sample size, national sampling, and has collected data on the brain imaging of 10,000+ American children44-48.

To understand the separate, additive, and multiplicative effects of the race (as a social factor) and age on the brain development of children, we borrowed data from a national sample of American children, with two aims: First to test the additive and multiplicative effects of age and race on children’s brain response to threat. By testing multiplicative effects, we mean to explore the differential associations between age and brain response to threat across racial groups. Using the ABCD data, we expected an interaction between race and age, which was an indicator of a difference between White and Black children for the effects of age on brain function. It is not race or age, but their combined and interactive effects that shape brain response to threat (negative face). This expectation is in line with the other research on behaviors and brain development showing that protective factors may have diminished effects for Black than White children49-51.

This cross-sectional study was a secondary analysis of the existing data, borrowed from the ABCD study44-48. This analysis is limited to the wave 1 data of the ABCD study. As described elsewhere44,52, ABCD is one of the largest brain imaging studies of child brain development that has ever been conducted. Among the main advantages of the ABCD study are being a national sample, a large sample size, a large sample of Blacks, public availability of the data, robust measurement of the brain development, and considerable psychiatric and psychosocial variables44-48.

Participants of the ABCD study were selected across multiple cities across states. This ABCD sample was primarily recruited through the school systems with sampling (school selection) informed by race, ethnicity, age, sex, socioeconomic status, and urbanicity. For additional methodological details of ABCD sampling and recruitment, please see the paper published elsewhere53. Analytical sample for this study was limited to White or Black children who had data on n-Back task (n=5,066).

Study constructs included age, race, ethnicity, sex, parental education (years), family income, financial difficulty, and neighborhood income and brain response to negative relative to neutral face (Table 1). PFC response to threatening (negative) relative to neutral face was captured by functional MRI measures, during the N-Back task. A detailed description of the ABCD study procedures and harmonization of the fMRI studies are available elsewhere45. N-Back task measures a wide range of brain functions such as emotion process, emotion regulation, impulsivity, working memory, encoding, retrieval, forgetting, and recognition. This task is ideal for measurement of dorsolateral prefrontal, parietal and premotor cortex, hippocampus, parahippocampus, and amygdala response.

Outcomes in this study were brain response to threatening (negative) relative to neutral faces. These outcomes were selected because brain response to threatening (negative) faces is shown to be a leading brain function that is essential for socialization. These included the mean beta weight for n-Back run 1 threatening (negative) face versus neutral face contrast in APARC ROI left hemisphere-lateral orbito-frontal, left hemisphere -superior-frontal, right hemisphere -caudal middle frontal, and right hemisphere -superior frontal. Table A lists the name of the outcome variables and their explanations.

Table A. The list of the outcome variables and their explanations.

Outcome

ABCD Study Variable

Variable

Task

Faces

Hemisphere

APARC ROI

Description

1

tfmri_nback_r1_757

Mean beta weight

n-Back run 1

Negative face versus neutral face contrast

Left

Lateral orbito-frontal

A part of the prefrontal cortex region, located in the frontal lobes of the brain, and involved in the cognitive processes such as decision-making.

2

tfmri_nback_r1_773

Mean beta weight

n-Back run 1

Negative face versus neutral face contrast

Left

Superior frontal

Also called as the marginal gyrus, is one of the frontal gyri, and makes up one third of the frontal lobe. This gyrus is bounded laterally by the superior frontal sulcus.

3

tfmri_nback_r1_783

Mean beta weight

n-Back run 1

Negative face versus neutral face contrast

Right

Caudal middle frontal

The caudal part of the middle frontal gyrus, which makes up about one-third of the frontal lobe of the human brain.

4

tfmri_nback_r1_807

Mean beta weight

n-Back run 1

Negative face versus neutral face contrast

Right

Superior frontal

A gyrus that continues onto the medial surface of the brain hemisphere. Cingulate sulcus is the main boundary between the superior frontal gyrus and the adjunct and underlying limbic lobe. It is also parallel to the corpus callosum before it turns upwards to the superior margin of the brain hemisphere under the name pars marginalis (marginal part).

Race. Race, a self-identified variable, was measured as bellow: Black = 1, White = 0 [reference]).

Age. Parents reported the age of the children. Age was calculated as the distance difference from date of birth to the date of the enrollment to the study. Age was a continuous measure in years.

Parents were asked, "What is the highest grade or level of school you have completed or the highest degree you have received?". This variable tanged from 1 (no official education) to 21 (doctoral degree).

Parents were asked "What is your total combined family income for the past 12 months? This should include income (before taxes and deductions) from all sources, wages, rent from properties, social security, disability and veteran's benefits, unemployment benefits, workman". Family income was a continuous measure ranging from 1 (Less than $5,000) to 10 ($200,000+), with a higher score indicating higher income.

Financial difficulties were measured by the following seven items: "In the past 12 months, has there been a time when you and your immediate family experienced any of the following:" 1)"Needed food but could not afford to buy it or could not afford to go out to get it?", 2) "Were without telephone service because you could not afford it?" 3)" Did not pay the full amount of the rent or mortgage because you could not afford it?", 4) "Were evicted from your home for not paying the rent or mortgage?", 5)"Had services turned off by the gas or electric company, or the oil company would not deliver oil because payments were not made?", 6) "Had someone who needed to see a doctor or go to the hospital but did not go because you could not afford it?", and 7) "Had someone who needed a dentist but could not go because you could not afford it?" Responses to each item were either 0 or 1. We calculated a sum score with a potential range between 0 and 7, a higher score indicating lower family socioeconomic status. Our variable was a continuous measure. Financial difficulties are an accepted socioeconomic status indicator54-60.

Derived from ABCD residential history file, we used the median family income of the neighborhood as neighborhood socioeconomic status. This is in line with the Area Deprivation Index (ADI), based on the work done by the Health Resources & Services Administration (HRSA), Amy Kind, Ana Diez Roux, and others. We used the neighborhood income of the county-level / census block group/neighborhood. Extensive research suggests that ADI, and median family income, and neighborhood income are predictors of health. This variable was not calculated in terms of 1 USD but 100,000 USD61-64.

Parents reported if they are of Hispanic ethnic background. This variable was coded as Hispanic = 1 and non-Hispanic = 0.

Sex was a dichotomous variable with males as 1 and females as 0.

We used SPSS for data analysis. For descriptive purposes, we reported frequency (%) and mean (standard deviation [SD]) for categorical and continuous variables, respectively. To conduct our bivariate analyses, we used Chi-square and independent samples t-test to compare Blacks and White children. To perform our multivariable analyses, we performed two series of four linear regressions for each outcome. The independent variable was age. The outcome were one of the structures namely 1) left hemisphere-lateral orbito-frontal cortex, 2) left hemisphere -superior-frontal cortex, 3) right hemisphere -caudal middle frontal cortex, and 4) right hemisphere -superior frontal cortex (Table 1). Our models first only controlled for sex. Then we ran models that also controlled for ethnicity and SES indicators namely parental education (years), family income, financial difficulty, and neighborhood income 100,000 USD. Model 1 was a pooled sample model without any interaction term, Model 2 was a pooled sample model with age by race interaction term, Model 3 was performed in White and Model 4 was performed in Black children. Identical models were run for each outcome, and without and with socioeconomic status covariates. Unstandardized regression coefficient (b), SE, and p-values were reported for each model. Given the explorative nature of this study, and given p values for interactions are larger, and interactions are hard to find, a p-value of equal or less than 0.1 was significant.

The ABCD study protocol received approval from the University of California, San Diego (UCSD) Institutional Review Board (cIRB). Some other universities that were a part of the ABCD study also obtained local IRB approval. All the participating children gave assent. Parents also signed informed consent. More detailed information on the ABCD study is available elsewhere52. Given that the data used in this study were fully de-identified, our investigation was non-human subject research. Thus, it found to be exempt from a full IRB review.

The current analysis was performed on 5,066 9-10 years old children who were either White (n = 3,782; %74.7) or Blacks (n = 1,284; %25.3). In Table 1 we have presented the descriptive demographic, socioeconomic status, and brain response to threat data. These data are presented for the pooled sample, as well as by race (Table 1).

As Table 1 shows, White and Black children did not differ in age and sex but differed in family socioeconomic status and neighborhood socioeconomic status. Compared to White children, Black children had lower SES. Black and White children did not differ in brain response to threat (Table 1).

Table 1. Descriptive data overall and by race.

 

All

(n = 5,066)

 Whites

(n = 3,782)

Blacks

(n = 1,284)

 

n

%

n

%

n

%

Race*a

 

 

 

 

 

 

White

3782

74.7

3782

100.0

-

-

Black

1284

25.3

-

-

1284

100.0

Ethnicity*a

 

 

 

 

 

 

Non-Hispanic

4276

84.4

3098

81.9

1178

91.7

Hispanic

790

15.6

684

18.1

106

8.3

Sex*a

 

 

 

 

 

 

Male

2431

48.0

1800

47.6

631

49.1

Female

2635

52.0

1982

52.4

653

50.9

 

Mean

SD

Mean

SD

Mean

SD

Age (Year)

9.44

0.50

9.43

0.50

9.45

0.51

Parental Education * b

16.80

2.50

17.25

2.34

15.46

2.49

Family income * b

7.18

2.46

7.86

1.98

5.20

2.67

Financial Difficulties * b

0.07

0.16

0.05

0.13

0.15

0.21

Neighborhood income * b

0.76

0.36

0.85

0.34

0.52

0.28

Activation of #1 in response to threatening than neutral faces

0.00

1.58

-0.02

1.58

0.06

1.59

Activation of #2 in response to threatening than neutral faces

-0.02

0.55

-0.02

0.46

-0.03

0.77

Activation of #3 in response to threatening than neutral faces

0.00

0.59

0.00

0.46

0.00

0.92

Activation of #4 in response to threatening than neutral faces

-0.01

0.55

-0.01

0.44

-0.01

0.82

Outcome #1: left hemisphere-lateral orbito-frontal, Outcome #2: left hemisphere -superior-frontal, Outcome #3: right hemisphere -caudal middle frontal, and Outcome #4: right hemisphere -superior frontal. All outcomes are mean beta weight for n-Back run 1 threatening face versus neutral face contrast. * p < 0.05 for comparison of Black and White people. a: Chi-Square test, b: independent samples t-test.

Table 2 presents the results of the unadjusted bivariate correlations based on the Pearson test. Socioeconomic status indicators were not correlated with brain response to threatening versus neutral faces. These correlations were inconsistent between racial groups and were based on brain outcome.

Table 2. Bivariate correlations in the pooled sample and by race (n=5,066).

 

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

All

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

1 Parental education

1

-.44**

.56**

-.00

-.02

-.02

-.04*

-.02

2 Family income

 

1

-.28**

.00

.02

-.02

-.01

-.03

3 Financial difficulties

 

 

1

.02

-.01

-.02

-.02

-.01

4 Neighborhood income

 

 

 

1

.01

-.00

.00

-.01

5 Activation of #1 in response to threatening than neutral faces

 

 

 

 

1

.10**

.05**

.08**

6 Activation of #2 in response to threatening than neutral faces

 

 

 

 

 

1

.79**

.93**

7 Activation of #3 in response to threatening than neutral faces

 

 

 

 

 

 

1

.85**

8 Activation of #4 in response to threatening than neutral faces

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

1

Whites

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

1 Parental education

1

-.44**

.48**

.01

.01

-.03

-.04*

-.03

2 Family income

 

1

-.22**

.01

.00

-.00

.01

-.02

3 Financial difficulties

 

 

1

.03

.01

-.02

-.01

.00

4 Neighborhood income

 

 

 

1

.03

.03

.03

.03

5 Activation of #1 in response to threatening than neutral faces

 

 

 

 

1

.16**

.10**

.15**

6 Activation of #2 in response to threatening than neutral faces

 

 

 

 

 

1

.72**

.91**

7 Activation of #3 in response to threatening than neutral faces

 

 

 

 

 

 

1

.78**

8 Activation of #4 in response to threatening than neutral faces

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

1

Blacks

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

1 Parental education

1

-.29**

.46**

.01

-.05

-.03

-.05

-.02

2 Family income

 

1

-.18**

-.01

.04

-.04

-.03

-.04

3 Financial difficulties

 

 

1

.04

-.05

-.05

-.06

-.05

4 Neighborhood income

 

 

 

1

-.05

-.07*

-.05

-.07

5 Activation of #1 in response to threatening than neutral faces

 

 

 

 

1

-.00

-.01

-.04

6 Activation of #2 in response to threatening than neutral faces

 

 

 

 

 

1

.87**

.95**

7 Activation of #3 in response to threatening than neutral faces

 

 

 

 

 

 

1

.91**

8 Activation of #4 in response to threatening than neutral faces

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

1

Outcome #1: left hemisphere-lateral orbito-frontal, Outcome #2: left hemisphere -superior-frontal, Outcome #3: right hemisphere -caudal middle frontal, and Outcome #4: right hemisphere -superior frontal. All outcomes are mean beta weight for n-Back run 1 threatening face versus neutral face contrast.

Table 3 reports the results of the two regression models in the pooled sample. Models 1-a, which only included the main effects, showed that age or race were not associated with activation of various brain regions in response to threatening than neutral faces. Model 2-a showed interactions between race and socioeconomic status on activation of the brain regions in response to threatening than neutral faces.

Table 3. Linear regressions in the pooled sample, without controlling for socioeconomic status (n = 5,066).

 

Model 1-a

All

Main Effects

 

Model 2-a

All

M1 + Interactions

 

 

B

p

b

P

Outcome: #1

 

 

 

 

Race (Black)

0.08

.205

2.27

.044

Age

0.03

.562

0.08

.146

Sex (male)

0.01

.873

0.01

.843

Age x Race

 

 

-0.23

.052

Outcome: #2

 

 

 

 

Race (Black)

-0.01

.622

1.27

.001

Age

0.00

.938

0.03

.126

Sex (male)

-0.01

.699

-0.01

.746

Age x Race

 

 

-0.14

.001

Outcome: #3

 

 

 

 

Race (Black)

0.00

.987

1.02

.016

Age

0.00

.914

0.03

.206

Sex (male)

0.00

.942

0.00

.979

Age x Race

 

 

-0.11

.016

Outcome: #4

 

 

 

 

Race (Black)

-0.01

.811

1.19

.002

Age

-0.01

.691

0.02

.257

Sex (male)

0.01

.748

0.01

.703

Age x Race

 

 

-0.13

.002

B: Unstandardized regression coefficient, SE: Standard Error, CI: Confidence Interval, Outcome #1: left hemisphere-lateral orbito-frontal, Outcome #2: left hemisphere -superior-frontal, Outcome #3: right hemisphere -caudal middle frontal, and Outcome #4: right hemisphere -superior frontal. All outcomes are mean beta weight for n-Back run 1 threatening face versus neutral face contrast.

Table 6 reports the results of race-specific models. Model 3-a was performed in White children and Model 4-b was performed in Black children. We found that age was not associated with brain response to threatening versus neutral faces in White or Black children.

Table 4. Linear regressions by race, without controlling for socioeconomic status (n=5,066).

 

Model 3-a Whites

 

Model 3-b Blacks

 

 

B

p

b

P

Outcome: #1

 

 

 

 

Age

0.01

.752

-0.17

.126

Sex (male)

0.10

.013

0.02

.878

Outcome: #2

 

 

 

 

Age

0.01

.496

-0.12

.089

Sex (male)

0.01

.722

-0.07

.338

Outcome: #3

 

 

 

 

Age

0.00

.950

-0.14

.083

Sex (male)

0.02

.304

-0.01

.865

Outcome: #4

 

 

 

 

Age

-0.01

.717

-0.15

.051

Sex (male)

0.00

.839

-0.04

.570

B: Unstandardized regression coefficient, SE: Standard Error, CI: Confidence Interval, Outcome #1: left hemisphere-lateral orbito-frontal, Outcome #2: left hemisphere -superior-frontal, Outcome #3: right hemisphere -caudal middle frontal, and Outcome #4: right hemisphere -superior frontal. All outcomes are mean beta weight for n-Back run 1 threatening face versus neutral face contrast.

Table 5 reports the results of two regression models in the pooled sample. Models 1-b, which only included the main effects showed that race or age were not associated with activation of various brain regions in response to threatening than neutral faces. Model 2-b showed interactions between race and socioeconomic status on activation of the brain regions in response to threatening versus neutral faces.

Table 5. Linear regressions in the pooled sample, while controlled for socioeconomic status (n=5,066).

 

Model 1-b

All

Main Effects

 

Model 2-b

All

M1 + Interactions

 

 

B

P

B

p

Outcome: #1

 

 

 

 

Race (Black)

0.15

.010

1.71

.071

Age

-0.03

.418

0.00

.996

Sex (male)

0.06

.110

0.06

.107

Ethnicity (Hispanic)

0.05

.407

0.05

.412

Parental education (years)

-0.01

.183

-0.01

.189

Family income

-0.02

.208

-0.02

.208

Financial difficulty

0.14

.359

0.14

.363

Neighborhood income (100000)

0.04

.558

0.04

.555

Age x Race

 

 

-0.16

.099

Outcome: #2

 

 

 

 

Race (Black)

0.00

.896

0.92

.058

Age

-0.02

.248

0.00

.840

Sex (male)

-0.01

.537

-0.01

.545

Ethnicity (Hispanic)

-0.02

.566

-0.02

.559

Parental education (years)

0.00

.645

0.00

.660

Family income

0.00

.620

0.00

.620

Financial difficulty

-0.13

.088

-0.13

.087

Neighborhood income (100000)

-0.06

.109

-0.06

.110

Age x Race

 

 

-0.10

.056

Outcome: #3

 

 

 

 

Race (Black)

-0.02

.559

0.98

.063

Age

-0.03

.208

-0.01

.765

Sex (male)

0.00

.991

0.00

.980

Ethnicity (Hispanic)

-0.03

.434

-0.03

.428

Parental education (years)

0.00

.646

0.00

.660

Family income

-0.01

.296

-0.01

.295

Financial difficulty

-0.12

.138

-0.13

.135

Neighborhood income (100000)

-0.06

.149

-0.06

.150

Age x Race

 

 

-0.11

.058

Outcome: # 4

 

 

 

 

Race (Black)

-0.01

.696

0.97

.048

Age

-0.04

.057

-0.02

.405

Sex (male)

-0.01

.635

-0.01

.645

Ethnicity (Hispanic)

-0.03

.373

-0.03

.367

Parental education (years)

0.00

.677

0.00

.692

Family income

-0.01

.393

-0.01

.392

Financial difficulty

-0.13

.086

-0.14

.084

Neighborhood income (100000)

-0.04

.228

-0.04

.230

Age x Race

 

 

-0.10

.045

B: Unstandardized regression coefficient, SE: Standard Error, CI: Confidence Interval, Outcome #1: left hemisphere-lateral orbito-frontal, Outcome #2: left hemisphere -superior-frontal, Outcome #3: right hemisphere -caudal middle frontal, and Outcome #4: right hemisphere -superior frontal. All outcomes are mean beta weight for n-Back run 1 threatening face versus neutral face contrast.

Table 6 reports the results of race-specific models. Model 3-b was performed in White children and Model 4-b was performed in Black children. We found that age was not associated with brain response to threatening versus neutral faces in White or Black children. However, the association was positive for Whites and threatening for Blacks.

Table 6. Linear regressions by race, while controlled for socioeconomic status (n=5,066).

 

Model 3-b

Whites

 

Model 4-b

Blacks

 

 

B

P

b

p

Outcome: # 1

 

 

 

 

Age

0.00

.955

-0.15

.162

Sex (male)

0.09

.032

-0.02

.849

Ethnicity (Hispanic)

0.11

.069

0.01

.946

Parental education (years)

0.00

.895

-0.05

.049

Family income

0.00

.966

-0.03

.321

Financial difficulty

0.30

.117

-0.01

.959

Neighborhood income (100000)

0.06

.460

-0.08

.699

Outcome: # 2

 

 

 

 

Age

-0.01

.763

-0.10

.154

Sex (male)

-0.01

.598

-0.02

.782

Ethnicity (Hispanic)

-0.02

.562

-0.05

.678

Parental education (years)

-0.01

.102

0.02

.225

Family income

0.00

.761

-0.01

.463

Financial difficulty

0.02

.853

-0.34

.063

Neighborhood income (100000)

-0.03

.377

-0.19

.167

Outcome: # 3

 

 

 

 

Age

-0.01

.666

-0.11

.143

Sex (male)

0.00

.825

0.02

.773

Ethnicity (Hispanic)

-0.03

.315

-0.03

.842

Parental education (years)

-0.01

.018

0.04

.051

Family income

0.00

.910

-0.03

.170

Financial difficulty

0.01

.900

-0.33

.092

Neighborhood income (100000)

-0.02

.604

-0.25

.095

Outcome: # 4

 

 

 

 

Age

-0.02

.280

-0.12

.100

Sex (male)

-0.01

.580

-0.01

.929

Ethnicity (Hispanic)

-0.04

.186

-0.04

.773

Parental education (years)

-0.01

.055

0.03

.150

Family income

0.00

.478

-0.01

.622

Financial difficulty

-0.03

.747

-0.30

.120

Neighborhood income (100000)

0.00

.978

-0.25

.089

B: Unstandardized regression coefficient, SE: Standard Error, CI: Confidence Interval, Outcome #1: left hemisphere-lateral orbito-frontal, Outcome #2: left hemisphere -superior-frontal, Outcome #3: right hemisphere -caudal middle frontal, and Outcome #4: right hemisphere -superior frontal. All outcomes are mean beta weight for n-Back run 1 threatening face versus neutral face contrast.

We found race by age effects in children brain response to threat. This is indicative of racial variation in age-related changes in brain response to threat. We, however, showed that these race by age effects remain significant when SES is controlled for. That is, race, per se, not as a proxy of SES, alters age-related brain development.

We found race per se not to be associated with brain response to threatening versus neutral faces in White but not Black children. Age was also not directly associated with activation of the brain in response to threatening versus neutral faces. Race and age, however, showed combined effects. Age would have a different implication for recognition and response to threat (negative emotion faces), with diminished salience of age for Black than White children. Although this was the main observed pattern for the outcomes, our four variables, response in the left hemisphere-lateral orbito-frontal, left hemisphere -superior-frontal, right hemisphere -caudal middle frontal, and right hemisphere -superior frontal ROIs also showed differences and nuances in their correlates.

Race was not found to change the brain response to threat. Race in our study was a proxy of adversities, stress, and poverty. Childhood poverty may be linked to reduced connectivity between the amygdala and hippocampus and some other regions, including the superior frontal cortex, lingual gyrus, posterior cingulate, and putamen65-68. The study showed that while childhood poverty predicts connectivity between 1) the left hippocampus and the right superior frontal cortex and 2) the right amygdala and the right lingual gyrus, this brain connectivity mediates the effect of childhood poverty on children depression69. Others have established hyperactivation of the reward network and hypoactivation of the executive network in low socioeconomic status individuals70. The effects of low socioeconomic status and stress on how the brain reacts to threat go beyond amygdala hyper-reactivity to threat and also impacts many other brain structures involved in memory and cognition71.

Research has shown that each additional year spent in poverty may be associated with a lower level of connectivity in neural networks involved in emotion regulation. However, these effects may be more pronounced for children who receive low levels of supportive parenting72. Blacks, compared to Whites, receive lower levels of protective parenting and live longer under poverty.

Our finding is indicative of double jeopardy for the brain development of Black children. The first risk is that race may be associated with poor brain development, as race is a proxy of exposure to poverty and racism. In addition, age-related brain development may be delayed, again, probably as a result of racism and discrimination, for Black children. Studies have shown that racial discrimination results in an increase in amygdala connectivity with multiple brain regions. In the presence of racial discrimination, the amygdala shows more strong connection with the thalamus. Discrimination increased the connections between the amygdala and the putamen, caudate, anterior insula, anterior cingulate, and medial frontal gyrus73.

The more salient effect of age for White than Black children’s brain development may be due to the lack of supportive elements and environment in Black lives. Other stressors such as racial and ethnic discrimination are unique in the life of racial minorities. Many studies have shown that social and physical environment is more enriched and resource filled for White than Black families. Libraries, schools, and houses where White families live have many books and educational resources. For Black families, however, such resources are scarce. As a result of growing in a rich or poor environment, age may not have the same effect on the brain development of White and Black children.

The results are related to a pattern called Minorities’ Diminished Returns (MDRs)50,51. MDRs refer to protective and risk factors that differently influence outcomes for Black and White families. For example, socioeconomic status interact with race on anxiety74, depression75, poor health76, poor school performance77,78, as well as high-risk behaviors49 such as aggression49 and tobacco use79,80. Black-White differences in correlates of health and development seem to be rules rather than exceptions76,81-84. For example, in the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study (FFCWS), correlates of impulsivity, school performance, school bonding, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), obesity, aggression, depression, and self-rated health differed for White and Black children84-87. These differences between Black and White children84-87 are due to differential effects of precursors on a wide range of developmental and health outcomes of Blacks and Whites76,81,83,88-92. As a result of these MDRs, Black children show diminished returns on health outcomes regardless of their profile of exposure to risk and protective factors, while for Whites, risk and protective effects alter the outcomes49,86,93-95. The same patterns of diminished returns are also shown for age96. Our study proposes that the same interaction may be seen for the effects of age on brain development.

Here we list six points that need caution. First, the use of age as a primary independent variable using cross-sectional data from a longitudinal cohort study may be inappropriate. There is some age variability, with a range of 8-11 years old, but this cohort was recruited intentionally to be roughly the same age. Future data releases will allow for within-person evaluations of longitudinal change in neural activation that will be better suited for answering the questions posed by this paper. Second, this study is simplistic as it used age as a primary independent variable using cross-sectional data from a longitudinal cohort study. Although there is some age variability, with a range of 8-11 years old, the ABCD cohort was recruited intentionally to be with low age range at baseline. Future data releases will allow for within-person evaluations of longitudinal change in neural activation that will be better suited for answering questions posed in this paper. Third, we should emphasize that we see race as a social factor (a proxy of poverty and socioeconomic status) on how the brain is affected by low or high socioeconomic status (parental education). Across various brain mechanisms, we focused on the amygdala, which is highly involved in emotional regulation, emotional expression, aggression, and impulsivity. An alteration of the amygdala response is expected to be involved in a wide range of emotional, cognitive, and behavioral outcomes. To be more specific, we tested if race, as a proxy of racism, adversities, and stress alters age-related brain development of children, with a specific focus on brain response to threatening (negative) relative to neutral face, which is shown to be one of the brain functions profoundly affected by race, socioeconomic status, stress, and adversity40,41,97-99. Fourth, this study was exploratory. The ABCD data provides an opportunity to answer important questions with high statistical power. Studies utilizing the ABCD data have the potential to have a major contribution to the field. This paper is the first step in this regard. Fifth, no matter how responsible we attempt to be in our interpretations, studies arguing for racial differences in biological functioning are controversial. They have the potential to be abused. They are at an increased risk of it being used to harm or stigmatize already marginalized groups. Sixth, in almost all epidemiological studies, race has an imbalanced distribution, and this has implications for the power of the study. Thus, the results of race-stratified models should be interpreted with caution. However, most of our analyses were in the pooled sample, which is less affected by the distribution of race.

In summary, in a large national sample of American children, age and race do not have independent (separate) but inter-dependent (multiplicative) effects on PFC response to threatening (negative) relative to neutral faces. This suggests that age-related brain development of children might be under the influence of race (as a social rather than biological construct). More research is needed on how social factors such as race, racism, social environment, adversities, and stress influence children brain development as a function of age.

Assari is supported by the NIH grants CA201415-02, 5S21MD000103, 54MD008149, R25 MD007610, 2U54MD007598, 4P60MD006923, and U54 TR001627. Multiple NIH funds ABCD study under awards.

Data used in the preparation of this article were obtained from the ABCD Study, held in the National Institutes of Mental Health Data Archive. The ABCD Study is supported by awards U01DA041022, U01DA041028, U01DA041048, U01DA041089, U01DA041106, U01DA041117, U01DA041120, U01DA041134, U01DA041148, U01DA041156, U01DA041174, U24DA041123, and U24DA041147 from the NIH and other federal partners. A full list of supporters is available at https://abcdstudy.org/federal-partners.html.

  1. Matsuzawa J, Matsui M, Konishi T, et al. Age-related volumetric changes of brain gray and white matter in healthy infants and children. Cereb Cortex. 2001; 11(4): 335-342.
  2. Czernochowski D, Mecklinger A, Johansson M. Age-related changes in the control of episodic retrieval: an ERP study of recognition memory in children and adults. Dev Sci. 2009; 12(6): 1026-1040.
  3. Danese A, Moffitt TE, Harrington H, et al. Adverse childhood experiences and adult risk factors for age-related disease: depression, inflammation, and clustering of metabolic risk markers. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2009; 163(12): 1135-1143.
  4. Van Strien JW, Glimmerveen JC, Franken IH, et al. Age-related differences in brain electrical activity during extended continuous face recognition in younger children, older children and adults. Dev Sci. 2011; 14(5): 1107-1118.
  5. Thormodsen R, Rimol LM, Tamnes CK, et al. Age-related cortical thickness differences in children with early-onset schizophrenia compared with healthy children. Psychiatry Res. 2013; 214(3): 190-196.
  6. Long X, Benischek A, Dewey D, et al. Age-related functional brain changes in young children. Neuroimage. 2017; 155: 322-330.
  7. Geneva IE, Krasteva MB, Kostianev SS. Age-related changes of the somatosensory evoked potentials in healthy children. Folia Med (Plovdiv). 2002; 44(4): 13-18.
  8. Lee SM, Choi YH, You SK, et al. Age-Related Changes in Tissue Value Properties in Children: Simultaneous Quantification of Relaxation Times and Proton Density Using Synthetic Magnetic Resonance Imaging. Invest Radiol. 2018; 53(4): 236-245.
  9. NIMH. The Teen Brain: 7 Things to Know. 2020.
  10. Mazza M, Tempesta D, Pino MC, et al. Regional cerebral changes and functional connectivity during the observation of negative emotional stimuli in subjects with post-traumatic stress disorder. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2013; 263(7): 575-583.
  11. Fernandes O, Jr., Portugal LCL, Alves RCS, et al. Decoding negative affect personality trait from patterns of brain activation to threat stimuli. Neuroimage. 2017; 145(Pt B): 337-345.
  12. Sylvester CM, Petersen SE, Luby JL, et al. Face processing in children with positive and negative threat bias. Psychol Med. 2017; 47(5): 800-809.
  13. Brown WJ, Wojtalik JA, Dewey D, et al. Affect and neural activity in women with PTSD during a task of emotional interference. J Affect Disord. 2016; 204: 9-15.
  14. Chien VS, Tsai AC, Yang HH, et al. Conscious and Non-conscious Representations of Emotional Faces in Asperger's Syndrome. J Vis Exp. 2016; (113).
  15. Demenescu LR, Kortekaas R, Cremers HR, et al. Amygdala activation and its functional connectivity during perception of emotional faces in social phobia and panic disorder. J Psychiatr Res. 2013; 47(8): 1024-1031.
  16. Ethofer T, Anders S, Erb M, et al. Impact of voice on emotional judgment of faces: an event-related fMRI study. Hum Brain Mapp. 2006; 27(9): 707-714.
  17. Emery NJ, Capitanio JP, Mason WA, et al. The effects of bilateral lesions of the amygdala on dyadic social interactions in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Behav Neurosci. 2001; 115(3): 515-544.
  18. Ball T, Derix J, Wentlandt J, et al. Anatomical specificity of functional amygdala imaging of responses to stimuli with positive and negative emotional valence. J Neurosci Methods. 2009; 180(1): 57-70.
  19. Klumpp H, Post D, Angstadt M, et al. Anterior cingulate cortex and insula response during indirect and direct processing of emotional faces in generalized social anxiety disorder. Biol Mood Anxiety Disord. 2013; 3: 7.
  20. Dannlowski U, Ohrmann P, Bauer J, et al. Amygdala reactivity to masked negative faces is associated with automatic judgmental bias in major depression: a 3 T fMRI study. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2007; 32(6): 423-429.
  21. Mingtian Z, Shuqiao Y, Xiongzhao Z, et al. Elevated amygdala activity to negative faces in young adults with early onset major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2012; 201(2): 107-112.
  22. Ruhe HG, Koster M, Booij J, et al. Occupancy of serotonin transporters in the amygdala by paroxetine in association with attenuation of left amygdala activation by negative faces in major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2014; 221(2): 155-161.
  23. Sanchez A, Romero N, De Raedt R. Depression-related difficulties disengaging from negative faces are associated with sustained attention to negative feedback during social evaluation and predict stress recovery. PLoS One. 2017; 12(3): e0175040.
  24. Blasi G, Hariri AR, Alce G, et al. Preferential amygdala reactivity to the negative assessment of neutral faces. Biol Psychiatry. 2009; 66(9): 847-853.
  25. Eastwood JD, Smilek D, Merikle PM. Differential attentional guidance by unattended faces expressing positive and negative emotion. Percept Psychophys. 2001; 63(6): 1004-1013.
  26. Kunar MA, Watson DG, Cole L, et al. Negative emotional stimuli reduce contextual cueing but not response times in inefficient search. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). 2014; 67(2): 377-393.
  27. Leist T, Dadds MR. Children' ability to read different emotional faces relates to their history of maltreatment and type of psychopathology. Clin Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009; 14(2): 237-250.
  28. Peterson BS, Rauh VA, Bansal R, et al. Effects of prenatal exposure to air pollutants (polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons) on the development of brain white matter, cognition, and behavior in later childhood. JAMA Psychiatry. 2015; 72(6): 531-540.
  29. Penton-Voak IS, Adams S, Button KS, et al. Emotional recognition training modifies neural response to emotional faces but does not improve mood in healthy volunteers with high levels of depressive symptoms. Psychol Med. 2020; 1-9.
  30. Isaacowitz DM, Allard ES, Murphy NA, et al. The time course of age-related preferences toward positive and threat. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2009; 64(2): 188-192.
  31. Franklin RG, Jr., Zebrowitz LA. Aging-Related Changes in Decoding Negative Complex Mental States from Faces. Exp Aging Res. 2016; 42(5): 471-478.
  32. Lagattuta KH, Kramer HJ. Try to look on the bright side: Children and adults can (sometimes) override their tendency to prioritize negative faces. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2017; 146(1): 89-101.
  33. Cloutier J, Li T, Correll J. The impact of childhood experience on amygdala response to perceptually familiar black and white faces. J Cogn Neurosci. 2014; 26(9): 1992-2004.
  34. Izuma K, Aoki R, Shibata K, et al. Neural signals in amygdala predict implicit prejudice toward an ethnic outgroup. Neuroimage. 2019; 189: 341-352.
  35. Masella RS, Meister M. The neuroanatomic basis of facial perception and variable facial discrimination ability: implications for orthodontics. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop. 2007; 132(3): 293-301.
  36. Senholzi KB, Depue BE, Correll J, et al. Brain activation underlying threat detection to targets of different races. Soc Neurosci. 2015; 10(6): 651-662.
  37. Telzer EH, Humphreys KL, Shapiro M, et al. Amygdala sensitivity to race is not present in childhood but emerges over adolescence. J Cogn Neurosci. 2013; 25(2): 234-244.
  38. Wheeler ME, Fiske ST. Controlling racial prejudice: social-cognitive goals affect amygdala and stereotype activation. Psychol Sci. 2005; 16(1): 56-63.
  39. Evans GW, Swain JE, King AP, et al. Childhood Cumulative Risk Exposure and Adult Amygdala Volume and Function. J Neurosci Res. 2016; 94(6): 535-543.
  40. Javanbakht A, Kim P, Swain JE, et al. Sex-Specific Effects of Childhood Poverty on Neurocircuitry of Processing of Emotional Cues: A Neuroimaging Study. Behav Sci (Basel). 2016; 6(4).
  41. Javanbakht A, King AP, Evans GW, et al. Childhood Poverty Predicts Adult Amygdala and Frontal Activity and Connectivity in Response to Emotional Faces. Front Behav Neurosci. 2015; 9: 154.
  42. Noble KG, Houston SM, Brito NH, et al. Family income, parental education and brain structure in children and children. Nature neuroscience. 2015; 18(5): 773.
  43. Pagliaccio D, Luby JL, Bogdan R, et al. HPA axis genetic variation, pubertal status, and sex interact to predict amygdala and hippocampus responses to negative emotional faces in school-age children. Neuroimage. 2015; 109: 1-11.
  44. Alcohol Research: Current Reviews Editorial S. NIH's Children Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study. Alcohol Res. 2018; 39(1): 97.
  45. Casey BJ, Cannonier T, Conley MI, et al. The Children Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study: Imaging acquisition across 21 sites. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2018; 32: 43-54.
  46. Karcher NR, O'Brien KJ, Kandala S, et al. Resting-State Functional Connectivity and Psychotic-like Experiences in Childhood: Results From the Children Brain Cognitive Development Study. Biol Psychiatry. 2019; 86(1): 7-15.
  47. Lisdahl KM, Sher KJ, Conway KP, et al. Children brain cognitive development (ABCD) study: Overview of substance use assessment methods. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2018; 32: 80-96.
  48. Luciana M, Bjork JM, Nagel BJ, et al. Children neurocognitive development and impacts of substance use: Overview of the children brain cognitive development (ABCD) baseline neurocognition battery. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2018; 32: 67-79.
  49. Assari S, Caldwell CH, Bazargan M. Association Between Parental Educational Attainment and Children Outcomes and Role of Race/Ethnicity. JAMA Netw Open. 2019; 2(11): e1916018.
  50. Assari S. Unequal Gain of Equal Resources across Racial Groups. Int J Health Policy Manag. 2017; 7(1): 1-9.
  51. Assari S. Health Disparities due to Diminished Return among Black Americans: Public Policy Solutions. Social Issues and Policy Review. 2018; 12(1): 112-145.
  52. Auchter AM, Hernandez Mejia M, Heyser CJ, et al. A description of the ABCD organizational structure and communication framework. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2018; 32: 8-15.
  53. Garavan H, Bartsch H, Conway K, et al. Recruiting the ABCD sample: Design considerations and procedures. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2018; 32: 16-22.
  54. Assari S, Smith J, Mistry R, et al. Substance Use among Economically Disadvantaged African American Older Adults; Objective and Subjective Socioeconomic Status. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2019; 16(10).
  55. Chen E, Paterson LQ. Neighborhood, family, and subjective socioeconomic status: How do they relate to children health? Health Psychol. 2006; 25(6): 704-714.
  56. Moon C. Subjective economic status, sex role attitudes, fertility, and mother's work. Ingu Pogon Nonjip. 1987; 7(1): 177-196.
  57. Assari S, Preiser B, Lankarani MM, et al. Subjective Socioeconomic Status Moderates the Association between Discrimination and Depression in African American Children. Brain Sci. 2018; 8(4).
  58. Boe T, Petrie KJ, Sivertsen B, et al. Interplay of subjective and objective economic well-being on the mental health of Norwegian children. SSM Popul Health. 2019; 9: 100471.
  59. Wright CE, Steptoe A. Subjective socioeconomic position, gender and cortisol responses to waking in an elderly population. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2005; 30(6): 582-590.
  60. Ye Z, Wen M, Wang W, et al. Subjective family socio-economic status, school social capital, and positive children development among young children in China: A multiple mediation model. Int J Psychol. 2020; 55(2): 173-181.
  61. Duran AC, Diez Roux AV, Latorre Mdo R, et al. Neighborhood socioeconomic characteristics and differences in the availability of healthy food stores and restaurants in Sao Paulo, Brazil. Health Place. 2013; 23: 39-47.
  62. Linetzky B, Mejia R, Ferrante D, et al. Socioeconomic status and tobacco consumption among children: a multilevel analysis of Argentina's Global Children Tobacco Survey. Nicotine Tob Res. 2012; 14(9): 1092-1099.
  63. Kim D, Diez Roux AV, Kiefe CI, et al. Do neighborhood socioeconomic deprivation and low social cohesion predict coronary calcification?: the CARDIA study. Am J Epidemiol. 2010; 172(3): 288-298.
  64. Nordstrom CK, Diez Roux AV, Jackson SA, et al. The association of personal and neighborhood socioeconomic indicators with subclinical cardiovascular disease in an elderly cohort. The cardiovascular health study. Soc Sci Med. 2004; 59(10): 2139-2147.
  65. Hair NL, Hanson JL, Wolfe BL, et al. Association of child poverty, brain development, and academic achievement. JAMA pediatrics. 2015; 169(9): 822-829.
  66. Dufford AJ, Kim P, Evans GW. The impact of childhood poverty on brain health: Emerging evidence from neuroimaging across the lifespan. In: International Review of Neurobiology. Elsevier. 2020; Vol 150 :77-105.
  67. Kolb B, Gibb R. Childhood poverty and brain development. Human Development. 2015 ;58(4-5): 215-217.
  68. Magnuson KA, Votruba-Drzal E. Enduring influences of childhood poverty. Citeseer; 2008.
  69. Barch D, Pagliaccio D, Belden A, et al. Effect of Hippocampal and Amygdala Connectivity on the Relationship Between Preschool Poverty and School-Age Depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2016; 173(6): 625-634.
  70. Yaple ZA, Yu R. Functional and Structural Brain Correlates of Socioeconomic Status. Cereb Cortex. 2019.
  71. Finn AS, Minas JE, Leonard JA, et al. Functional brain organization of working memory in children varies in relation to family income and academic achievement. Dev Sci. 2017; 20(5).
  72. Brody GH, Yu T, Nusslock R, et al. The Protective Effects of Supportive Parenting on the Relationship Between Children Poverty and Resting-State Functional Brain Connectivity During Adulthood. Psychol Sci. 2019:956797619847989.
  73. Clark US, Miller ER, Hegde RR. Experiences of Discrimination Are Associated With Greater Resting Amygdala Activity and Functional Connectivity. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2018; 3(4): 367-378.
  74. Assari S, Caldwell CH, Zimmerman MA. Family Structure and Subsequent Anxiety Symptoms; Minorities' Diminished Return. Brain Sci. 2018; 8(6).
  75. Assari S, Caldwell CH. High Risk of Depression in High-Income African American Boys. J Racial Ethn Health Disparities. 2018; 5(4): 808-819.
  76. Assari S. Parental Educational Attainment and Mental Well-Being of College Students; Diminished Returns of Blacks. Brain Sci. 2018; 8(11).
  77. Assari S. Parental Educational Attainment and Academic Performance of American College Students; Blacks’ Diminished Returns. Journal of Health Economics and Development. 2019; 1(1): 21-31.
  78. Assari S, Caldwell CH. Parental Educational Attainment Differentially Boosts School Performance of American Children: Minorities' Diminished Returns. J Family Reprod Health. 2019; 13(1): 7-13.
  79. Assari S, Mistry R. Educational Attainment and Smoking Status in a National Sample of American Adults; Evidence for the Blacks' Diminished Return. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2018; 15(4).
  80. Assari S, Mistry R, Bazargan M. Race, Educational Attainment, and E-Cigarette Use. Journal of Medical Research and Innovation. 2020; 4(1): e000185-e000185.
  81. Assari S. Parental Education Attainment and Educational Upward Mobility; Role of Race and Gender. Behav Sci (Basel). 2018; 8(11).
  82. Assari S, Moghani Lankarani M. Poverty Status and Childhood Asthma in White and Black Families: National Survey of Children's Health. Healthcare (Basel). 2018; 6(2).
  83. Assari S. Race, Intergenerational Social Mobility and Stressful Life Events. Behav Sci (Basel). 2018; 8(10).
  84. Assari S. Multiplicative Effects of Social and Psychological Risk Factors on College Students' Suicidal Behaviors. Brain Sci. 2018; 8(5).
  85. Assari S. Socioeconomic Status and Self-Rated Oral Health; Diminished Return among Hispanic Whites. Dent J (Basel). 2018; 6(2).
  86. Assari S, Thomas A, Caldwell CH, et al. Blacks' Diminished Health Return of Family Structure and Socioeconomic Status; 15 Years of Follow-up of a National Urban Sample of Children. J Urban Health. 2018; 95(1): 21-35.
  87. Assari S, Caldwell CH. Family Income at Birth and Risk of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder at Age 15: Racial Differences. Children (Basel). 2019; 6(1).
  88. Assari S, Lankarani MM, Caldwell CH. Does Discrimination Explain High Risk of Depression among High-Income African American Men? Behav Sci (Basel). 2018; 8(4).
  89. Fuller-Rowell TE, Doan SN. The social costs of academic success across ethnic groups. Child Dev. 2010; 81(6): 1696-1713.
  90. Fuller-Rowell TE, Curtis DS, Doan SN, et al. Racial disparities in the health benefits of educational attainment: a study of inflammatory trajectories among African American and white adults. Psychosom Med. 2015; 77(1): 33-40.
  91. Hudson DL, Bullard KM, Neighbors HW, et al. Are benefits conferred with greater socioeconomic position undermined by racial discrimination among African American men? J Mens Health. 2012; 9(2): 127-136.
  92. Hudson DL, Neighbors HW, Geronimus AT, et al. The relationship between socioeconomic position and depression among a US nationally representative sample of African Americans. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2012; 47(3): 373-381.
  93. Assari S, Caldwell CH, Mincy R. Family Socioeconomic Status at Birth and Children Impulsivity at Age 15; Blacks' Diminished Return. Children (Basel). 2018; 5(5).
  94. Assari S, Caldwell CH, Mincy RB. Maternal Educational Attainment at Birth Promotes Future Self-Rated Health of White but Not Black Children: A 15-Year Cohort of a National Sample. J Clin Med. 2018; 7(5).
  95. Assari S, Boyce S, Bazargan M, et al. Unequal Protective Effects of Parental Educational Attainment on the Body Mass Index of Black and White Children. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2019; 16(19): 3641.
  96. Chalian H, Khoshpouri P, Assari S. Patients’ age and discussion with doctors about lung cancer screening: Diminished returns of Blacks. AGING MEDICINE. 2019; 2(1): 35-41.
  97. D'Angiulli A, Lipina SJ, Olesinska A. Explicit and implicit issues in the developmental cognitive neuroscience of social inequality. Front Hum Neurosci. 2012; 6: 254.
  98. Kim P, Evans GW, Angstadt M, et al. Effects of childhood poverty and chronic stress on emotion regulatory brain function in adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013; 110(46): 18442-18447.
  99. Silverman ME, Muennig P, Liu X, et al. The impact of socioeconomic status on the neural substrates associated with pleasure. Open Neuroimag J. 2009; 3: 58-63.
 

Article Info

Article Notes

  • Published on: October 12, 2020

Keywords

  • Age

  • Age-related
  • Development
  • Socioeconomic status
  • Socioeconomic position
  • Amygdala
  • Limbic system
  • Negative emotion
  • Emotion regulation
  • Brain development
  • fMRI

*Correspondence:

Dr. Shervin Assari
Department of Family Medicine, Charles R. Drew University of Medicine and Science, Los Angeles, CA, USA; Telephone No: 1-734-232-0445; Fax No: +734-615-8739
Email: assari@umich.edu.